Why you are never satisfied? Researchers may explain

New research by Daniel Gilbert speaks to our conflicted relationship with progress.

Share

By a few measures, including rates of poverty and violence, advance is a worldwide reality. Why, at that point, do as such a large number of us accept something else?

The answer, Harvard researcher Daniel Gilbert says, may lie in “prevalence-induced concept change.”

In a series of studies, Gilbert along with colleagues, demonstrate that as the prevalence of a problem is decreased, people are slanted to rethink the issue. As an issue becomes smaller, conceptualizations of the issue extensively, which can prompt advance being marked down.

Even if warning of the tendency or offering money to avoid it, they continued to alter their definitions of blue.

Gilbert said,”Our studies show that people judge each new instance of a concept in the context of the previous instances. So as we reduce the prevalence of a problem, such as discrimination, for example, we judge each new behavior in the improved context that we have created.”

“Another way to say this is that solving problems causes us to expand our definitions of them. When problems become rare, we count more things as problems. Our studies suggest that when the world gets better, we become harsher critics of it, and this can cause us to mistakenly conclude that it hasn’t actually gotten better at all. Progress, it seems, tends to mask itself.”

“The phenomenon isn’t limited to large, seemingly intractable social issues. In several experiments described in the paper, it emerged even when participants were merely asked to look for blue dots.”

“We had volunteers look at thousands of dots on a computer screen one at a time and decide if each was or was not blue. We lowered the prevalence of blue dots, and what we found was that our participants began to classify as blue dots they had previously classified as purple.”

Another test, this one utilizing faces, indicated comparable outcomes. At the point when the prevalence of undermining faces was lessened, individuals started to recognize neutral faces as threatening.

Gilbert said, “We asked participants to review proposals for studies that varied from highly ethical to highly unethical,” he said. “Over time, we lowered the prevalence of unethical studies, and sure enough, when we did that, our participants started to identify innocuous studies as unethical.”

“Sometimes this prevalence-induced concept change and makes sense.”

“If the ER is full of gunshot victims and someone comes in with a broken arm, the doctor will tell that person to wait. But imagine one Sunday where there are no gunshot victims. Should that doctor hold her definition of ‘needing immediate attention’ constant and tell the guy with the broken arm to wait anyway? Of course not. She should change her definition based on this new context.”

In other cases, however, prevalence-induced concept change can be a problem.

Gilbert said, “Nobody thinks a radiologist should change his definition of what constitutes a tumor and continue to find them even when they’re gone. That’s a case in which you really must be able to know when your work is done. You should be able to see that the prevalence of tumors has gone to zero and call it a day. Our studies simply suggest that this isn’t an easy thing to do.”

Daniel Gilbert, professor of psychology and author of "Stumbling on Happiness," and host of the PBS television series "This Emotional Life,"speaks on 2/16 on the science of decisionmaking. "How To Do Precisely the Right Thing at All Possible Times" will explore new research in psychology, neuroscience, and behavioral economics. Jon Chase/Harvard Staff Photographer
Daniel Gilbert, professor of psychology and author of “Stumbling on Happiness,” and host of the PBS television series “This Emotional Life,”speaks on 2/16 on the science of decisionmaking. “How To Do Precisely the Right Thing at All Possible Times” will explore new research in psychology, neuroscience, and behavioral economics. Jon Chase/Harvard Staff Photographer

Aside from the obvious questions the research raises about problem-solving, it also suggests challenges with how we discuss, debate, and address societal ills.

“Expanding one’s definition of a problem may be seen by some as evidence of political correctness run amok. They will argue that reducing the prevalence of discrimination, for example, will simply cause us to start calling more behaviors discriminatory. Others will see the expansion of concepts as an increase in social sensitivity, as we become aware of problems that we previously failed to recognize.

“Our studies take no position on this,.There are clearly times in life when our definitions should be held constant, and there are clearly times when they should be expanded. Our experiments simply show that when we are in the former circumstance, we often act as though we are in the latter.”

The research is described in a paper in the June 29 issue of Science.

- Advertisement -

Latest Updates

Trending